Emerging Technologies

Can birdsong help us understand human language?

Robin Ann Smith
Journalist, Duke University
Share:
Our Impact
What's the World Economic Forum doing to accelerate action on Emerging Technologies?
The Big Picture
Explore and monitor how Innovation is affecting economies, industries and global issues
A hand holding a looking glass by a lake
Crowdsource Innovation
Get involved with our crowdsourced digital platform to deliver impact at scale
Stay up to date:

Innovation

The song of the swamp sparrow — a grey-breasted bird found in wetlands throughout much of North America — is a simple melodious trill, repeated over and over again.

“It’s kind of like a harmonious police whistle,” said biologist Stephen Nowicki.

But according to a new study by Duke University scientists Nowicki and Robert Lachlan, swamp sparrows are capable of processing the notes that make up their simple songs in more sophisticated ways than previously realized — an ability that may help researchers better understand the perceptual building blocks that enable language in humans.

The study appears in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

From the finite types of sounds that make up a stream of speech — such as the “c” sound in “cat” or the “b” sound in “boy” — humans are able to create and make sense of an almost infinite number of words and sentences about the present, past and future, unconsciously and automatically.

What’s more, how humans perceive speech sounds is influenced by context, said Lachlan, now of Queen Mary University of London.

In American English, for example, listeners recognize that the “t” in “city” and the “d” in “ready” belong to different categories, even though they’re frequently pronounced the same.

Lachlan and Nowicki wanted to know if this common aspect of understanding spoken language, called partial phonemic overlapping, is found in birds, too.

To find out, they recorded and analyzed the songs of 206 male swamp sparrows near Pymatuning Lake in Pennsylvania.

Statistical analysis revealed that the short repeated syllables that make up each song consist of subsets of roughly 10 types of notes.

In two experiments, the researchers compared males’ territorial responses to songs in which either the first note or the last note of each syllable was substituted with a note of a different type — either short, intermediate or long.

How the birds perceived a particular note depended on where it fell in a snippet of song.

The birds responded to the modified songs with an aggressive territorial display when the note substitution occurred in one position in the snippet, but much more weakly or not at all when the same note was substituted in another position — indicating that the birds are able to assign the same sound to different categories of notes depending on the context in which it appears.

The study is part of a larger body of research aimed at understanding how language arose in humans by studying different forms of communication in animals.

Human language draws on a complex set of cognitive skills, some of which are also found in songbirds. That fact alone is not entirely surprising to scientists, especially in light of recent research led by Duke’s Erich Jarvis showing that songbirds and humans rely on many of the same genes to sing and speak.

But what’s exciting about their results, the researchers say, is it suggests that the ability to perceive speech sounds differently in different contexts — a critical skill necessary for the perception of human speech — could have arisen before, rather than after, other aspects of human language such as semantics and syntax came to be.

This research was funded by Duke University and an Arthur and Barbara Pape Award from the University of Pittsburgh.

This article is published in collaboration with Duke Today. Publication does not imply endorsement of views by the World Economic Forum.

To keep up with Forum:Agenda subscribe to our weekly newsletter.

Author: Robin Smith was a researcher and writing teacher for more than ten years before joining the news office at Duke University, where she covers the life and physical sciences across campus.

Image: Sparrows cool down in a pond during unusually warm weather as temperatures reached 25 degrees Celsius (77 degrees Fahrenheit) in Skopje October 25, 2013. REUTERS/Ognen Teofilovski.

Don't miss any update on this topic

Create a free account and access your personalized content collection with our latest publications and analyses.

Sign up for free

License and Republishing

World Economic Forum articles may be republished in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International Public License, and in accordance with our Terms of Use.

The views expressed in this article are those of the author alone and not the World Economic Forum.

Share:
World Economic Forum logo
Global Agenda

The Agenda Weekly

A weekly update of the most important issues driving the global agenda

Subscribe today

You can unsubscribe at any time using the link in our emails. For more details, review our privacy policy.

‘AI will likely make drugs cheaper and more accessible for everybody on the planet’ – 3 technologists on AI and scientific discovery

David Elliott

July 5, 2024

About Us

Events

Media

Partners & Members

  • Join Us

Language Editions

Privacy Policy & Terms of Service

© 2024 World Economic Forum