Education and Skills

Can economic reasoning play a role in cognitive science?

Economic incentives may help explain the 'Flynn effect'.

Economic incentives may help explain the 'Flynn effect'. Image: Unsplash/Natasha Connell

Santiago Hermo
PhD student in Economics, Brown University
Miika Päällysaho
Ph.D. student in Economics, Stockholm University
David Seim
Professor of Economics, Stockholm University
Jesse Shapiro
George Gund Professor of Economics and Business Administration, Harvard University
  • A longstanding puzzle in cognitive science is the 'Flynn effect' - a secular rise in reasoning skills across generations.
  • Cognitive scientists have emphasized explanations such as improvements in health and nutrition.
  • But researchers argue the effect may partially originate from changes in economic incentives.
  • Estimates suggest about a third of the rise in reasoning skills is attributable to changes in workplace tasks, with analytically intensive occupations growing in importance.

A longstanding puzzle in cognitive science is the secular rise in cognitive skill across generations. Cognitive scientists emphasise explanations such as improvements in health and nutrition. This column describes research using an economic model and population administrative data from Sweden to argue that this ‘Flynn effect’ may originate in part from changes in economic incentives. Estimates suggest about a third of the rise in reasoning skill over the period studied is attributable to changes in labour market returns to different skills, with analytically intensive occupations having grown in relative importance.

Figure 1 shows trends in performance on a logical reasoning task among men in Sweden born between 1962 and 1975. The data come from tests taken at military enlistment around age 18 or 19. Because enlistment was compulsory and the tests were unchanged over time, these data give us an unusually clear picture of population trends in cognitive skill for men.

The figure shows a sharp rise in logical reasoning skill, implying that an average man born in 1975 scored 4.4 percentile points better than one born in 1962. This corresponds to a rate of change of 8.5 percentile points per 25-year generation.

Figure 1 Trends in logical reasoning skill among Swedish men born in 1962–1975

Average logical reasoning skill level Swedish men Flynn effect
An average man born in 1975 scored 4.4 percentile points better than one born in 1962. Image: Adapted from Figure I of Hermo et al. (forthcoming)

Notes: The figure depicts the average logical reasoning score for each birth cohort, expressed as a percentile of the 1967 birth cohort, based on tests taken at military enlistment around age 18 or 19.

The secular rise in ‘fluid intelligence’, or reasoning skill, has been called the ‘Flynn effect’ in honour of James Flynn, who did pioneering work on this topic beginning in the early 1980s (Flynn 1984, 2007, 2012). As Figure 2 shows, the Flynn effect is a very broad phenomenon, occurring across many developed countries (Pietschnig and Voracek 2015). It has been documented before using Swedish military enlistment data, for example by Rönnlund et al. (2013), and is related to patterns documented in other contexts, for example by Jokela et al. (2017).

Figure 2 Measured trends in fluid IQ in selected studies

The Flynn effect is a very broad phenomenon, occurring across many developed countries.
The Flynn effect is a very broad phenomenon, occurring across many developed countries. Image: Adapted from Online Appendix Figure 9 of Hermo et al. (forthcoming)

Notes: The figure depicts the annual change in fluid IQ points for each of a set of country studies, drawn either from Pietschnig and Voracek (2015, Table S1, circles) or from the military enlistment sample that we study (triangle).

What explains this pattern? Cognitive scientists have tended to emphasise factors, such as improvements in health and nutrition, that economists would recognise as increasing the supply of skill (e.g. Pietschnig and Voracek 2015, Rindermann et al. 2017). But these explanations have proved incomplete, in part because it is difficult to find evidence of sufficient changes in health, nutrition, and other factors that can explain such large gains in test performance in contexts, such as Sweden, with highly developed economies and comparatively healthy populations. For this reason, the Flynn effect is viewed by some as an enduring puzzle. One authoritative review, for example, says that “[i]f there were a prize in the field of human intelligence research, it might be for the person who can explain the ‘Flynn effect’….” (Deary 2020).

In a forthcoming article (Hermo et al. forthcoming), we study whether economic incentives have a role to play in explaining the evolution of cognitive skills across birth cohorts.

A role for economic incentives?

Figure 3 shows trends in the estimated labour market return to a one-percentile-point increase in measured fluid intelligence across the birth cohorts that we study. In line with prior research (Castex and Dechter 2014, Markussen and Røed 2020, Edin et al. 2022), we find that this return declined across cohorts.

Figure 3 Trends in labour market returns to logical reasoning skill among Swedish men born in 1962–1975

Lifetime skill premium among Swedish men born in 1962–1975. Flynn effect
The estimated labor market return declined over the years studied. Image: Adapted from Figure I of Hermo et al. (forthcoming)

Notes: The figure depicts the estimated lifetime skill premium, expressed in natural logarithms, for a one-percentile-point increase in logical reasoning skill for each birth cohort.

Taken on their own, Figures 1 and 3 seem to bear out the emphasis in cognitive science on factors that change the supply of skills. An increase in the supply of cognitive skill – say, due to improvements in health or nutrition – would increase the quantity of skill (Figure 1) and decrease its market value (Figure 3), very much in line with the patterns we observe.

The picture changes, however, when we examine a second dimension of cognitive skill. Figure 4 presents trends in the level of vocabulary knowledge across the cohorts that we study. Vocabulary knowledge is a common measure of ‘crystallised intelligence’, or domain knowledge, which is often found to have grown more slowly than fluid intelligence, or even (as we see for Sweden), declined across cohorts.

Figure 4 Trends in vocabulary knowledge among Swedish men born in 1962–1975

Vocabulary knowledge Swedish men Flynn effect
Vocabulary knowledge is often found to have grown more slowly than fluid intelligence or declined. Image: Adapted from Figure I of Hermo et al. (forthcoming)

Notes: The figure depicts the average vocabulary knowledge score for each birth cohort, expressed as a percentile of the 1967 birth cohort, based on tests taken at military enlistment around age 18 or 19.

The decline in crystallised intelligence suggests forces at work beyond generalised improvements to health, nutrition, and other supply-side factors. Could incentives play a role?

Figure 5 suggests that they might. The figure plots the change in the relative level of logical reasoning and vocabulary knowledge performance against their relative return. The two are strongly positively associated across cohorts, suggesting the possibility that the level of different skills is adjusting to their different returns in the labour market.

Figure 5 Co-evolution of relative skill levels and labour market returns

Relative skill levels labor market returns Flynn effect
The decline in crystallized intelligence suggests that incentives could play a role. Image: Figure II of Hermo et al. (forthcoming)

Notes: The figure shows a scatterplot of the natural logarithm of the relative average skill levels against the natural logarithm of the relative estimated lifetime skill premia for each birth cohort. The dashed line depicts the line of best fit.

An economic model of skill investment

We have developed an economic model of skill investment (Hermo et al. forthcoming). In the model, members of a given cohort make skill investment decisions taking into account their expectations of future labour market returns to different skills. We show how to use the data in Figures 1, 3, 4, and 5 to estimate the key parameters of the model, and thereby to decompose trends in skill levels into components driven by changes in economic incentives (as proxied by labour market returns) and changes in the supply of skills (due to health, nutrition, and the like).

Figure 6 shows that, according to the model, logical reasoning skill would have risen even in the absence of changes in labour market returns, but by only 63% as much as it did in reality. In this sense, the model implies that changes in labour market returns explain 37% of the Flynn effect in our setting.

Figure 6 Decomposing the cohort trends in logical reasoning skill

The model implies that changes in labour market returns explain 37% of the Flynn effect in our setting.
The model implies that changes in labour market returns explain 37% of the Flynn effect in our setting. Image: Adapted from Figure IV of Hermo et al. (forthcoming)

Notes: The “Actual” series depicts the average logical reasoning score for each birth cohort, expressed as a percentile of the 1967 birth cohort, based on tests taken at military enlistment around age 18 or 19. The “Holding labour market returns constant” series depicts the counterfactual series if skill premia remained constant at the level estimated for the 1962 birth cohort.

The role of parents and schools

According to our analysis, skill investments respond to economic incentives. But who is responding? We investigated the roles of two important actors in the skill investment process: parents and schools.

Figure 7, which uses data from an original survey of Swedish parents, shows that parents of more recent cohorts of children tend to regard reasoning as more important than knowledge, as compared to parents of earlier cohorts. Although Figure 7 does not prove that changes in parents’ emphasis arise from changes in economic incentives, it does suggest that parents’ emphasis on different skill dimensions changes over time.

Figure 7 Trends in parents’ perceptions of the importance of reasoning versus knowledge

Parents' perceptions importance of reasoning versus knowledge child Flynn effect
Parents of more recent cohorts of children tend to regard reasoning as more important than knowledge. Image: Adapted from Figure VI of Hermo et al. (forthcoming)

Notes: Data come from an original survey of parents’ perceptions. The figure shows the fraction of respondents rating reasoning as more important (red bars), knowledge as more important (green bars), or reasoning and knowledge as equally important (blue bars), separately by decile of the birth cohort of the respondent’s first child.

Figure 8 shows similar evidence for schools, using data from an original text analysis of Swedish primary school curricula. Terms relating to reasoning or analysis have become more common over time than terms relating to knowledge or memorisation, in line with a large body of pedagogical research that studies these curricula qualitatively (Larsson 2011, Johansson and Wickman 2012, Prytz 2015, Dahlbäck and Lyngfelt 2017).

Figure 8 Trends in emphasis on reasoning versus knowledge in Swedish primary school curricula

Average exposure to reasoning vs knowledge by year of birth between 1962 and 1975. Flynn effect
Terms relating to reasoning or analysis have become more common over time than terms relating to knowledge or memorization. Image: Figure VII of Hermo et al. (forthcoming)

Notes: The figure shows the trend across birth cohorts in the emphasis on reasoning versus knowledge in the Swedish primary school curricula prevailing during each cohort’s primary schooling. To construct the figure, we first associate different editions of the curriculum to the cohort’s primary schooling years, then calculate the ratio of the number of appearances of keywords related to reasoning versus knowledge for each edition, and then calculate the average exposure to reasoning versus knowledge as the average of the ratio of keyword appearances over the cohort’s primary school years.

Discover

What is the World Economic Forum doing to improve digital intelligence in children?

Changes in the demand for skills

James Flynn wrote that “[w]hen society asks us to increase our use of any skill over time, the brain responds” (2018: 79). Figure 5 suggests that, over time, the labour market is placing increasing emphasis on reasoning skill as opposed to knowledge. Why?

Analysis we report in our article suggests that this trend may originate in part with changes in the nature of tasks in the workplace (Hermo et al. forthcoming). Occupations that are especially intensive analytically (such as those relating to computers) have grown at the expense of others (such as sales work) that are more demanding in terms of domain knowledge.

Conclusions

The stock of human capital plays an important role in economic growth and prosperity (e.g. Bishop 1989, Toivanen and Väänänen 2013, Aghion et al. 2017). Cognitive scientists studying population trends in measured intelligence have tended to emphasise the role of factors affecting the supply of skills. Our analysis suggests that the mix of skills in a society also evolves in response to the society’s demands and suggests the value of economic reasoning in the study of population trends in measured intelligence.

Have you read?

References

Aghion, P, U Akcigit, A Hyytinen and O Toivanen (2017), “The social origins and IQ of inventors”, VoxEU.org, 23 December.

Bishop, J H (1989), “Is the Test Score Decline Responsible for the Productivity Growth Decline?”, American Economic Review 79(1): 178–197.

Castex, G and E Kogan Dechter (2014), “The Changing Roles of Education and Ability in Wage Determination”, Journal of Labor Economics 32(4): 685–710.

Dahlbäck, K and A Lyngfelt (2017), “Estetiska dimensioner i svenskämnets kursplaner från Lgr 69 till Lgr 11”, Educare 1: 152–182.

Deary, I J (2020), Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction, 2nd ed, New York: Oxford University Press.

Edin, P-A, P Fredriksson, M Nybom and B Öckert (2022), “The Rising Return to Noncognitive Skill”, American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 14(2): 78–100.

Flynn, J R (1984), “The Mean IQ of Americans: Massive Gains 1932 to 1978”, Psychological Bulletin 95(1): 29–51.

Flynn, J R (2007), What is Intelligence? Beyond the Flynn Effect, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Flynn, J R (2012), Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Flynn, J R (2018), “Reflections About Intelligence Over 40 Years”, Intelligence 70: 73–83.

Hermo, S, M Päällysaho, D Seim and J M Shapiro (forthcoming), “Labor Market Returns and the Evolution of Cognitive Skills: Theory and Evidence,” Quarterly Journal of Economics.

Johansson, A-M and P-O Wickman (2012), “Vad ska elever lära sig angående naturvetenskaplig verksamhet? En analys av svenska läroplaner för grundskolan under 50 år”, Nordic Studies in Science Education 8(3): 197–212.

Jokela, M, T Pekkarinen, M Sarvimäki, M Terviö and R Uusitalo (2017), “Secular rise in economically valuable personality traits”, VoxEU.org, 11 July.

Larsson, H A (2011), Mot bättre vetande: En svensk skolhistoria, Stockholm: SNS Förlag.

Markussen, S and K Røed (2020), “Economic Mobility Under Pressure”, Journal of the European Economic Association 18(4): 1844–1885.

Pietschnig, J and M Voracek (2015), “One Century of Global IQ Gains: A Formal Meta-analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909–2013)”, Perspectives on Psychological Science 10(3): 282–306.

Prytz, J (2015), “Swedish Mathematics Curricula, 1850-2014: An Overview”, Proceedings of the Third International Conference on the History of Mathematics Education 309–326.

Rindermann, H, D Becker, and T R Coyle (2017), “Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: The FLynn Effect and the Future of Intelligence”, Personality and Individual Differences 106: 242–247.

Rönnlund, M, B Carlstedt, Y Blomstedt, L-G Nilsson and L Weinehall (2013), “Secular Trends in Cognitive Test Performance: Swedish Conscript Data 1970–1993”, Intelligence 41(1): 19–24.

Toivanen, O and L Väänänen (2013), “Does education lead to more innovation?”, VoxEU.org, 21 July.

Loading...
Don't miss any update on this topic

Create a free account and access your personalized content collection with our latest publications and analyses.

Sign up for free

License and Republishing

World Economic Forum articles may be republished in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International Public License, and in accordance with our Terms of Use.

The views expressed in this article are those of the author alone and not the World Economic Forum.

Stay up to date:

Sweden

Related topics:
Education and SkillsEconomic Growth
Share:
The Big Picture
Explore and monitor how Neuroscience is affecting economies, industries and global issues
World Economic Forum logo

Forum Stories newsletter

Bringing you weekly curated insights and analysis on the global issues that matter.

Subscribe today

13 leaders on the books that changed how they work, live and lead

David Elliott

December 19, 2024

From classroom to career: Building a future-ready global workforce

About us

Engage with us

  • Sign in
  • Partner with us
  • Become a member
  • Sign up for our press releases
  • Subscribe to our newsletters
  • Contact us

Quick links

Language editions

Privacy Policy & Terms of Service

Sitemap

© 2024 World Economic Forum