Education and Skills

Here’s how racial stereotypes can affect a student’s success in STEM

A Muslim student from the Chinese Hui minority studies at Ningxia Islamic College in Yinchuan, capital of northwest China's Ningxia Hui Autonomous Region, April 23,2007. According to school and local sources, more than 300 students, including 82 girls, learn culture, Arabic language, Islam and history at the college, which was founded in 1989.  REUTERS/Jason Lee (CHINA) - GM1DVCJKDWAA

According to research, looks do matter. Image: REUTERS/Jason Lee

Amy McCaig
Senior Media Relations Specialist, Rice University

The researchers did not look at differences across races. Rather, they focused on differences within races.

Examining five years’ worth of students who entered college intending to pursue a STEM degree, the researchers measured their persistence at remaining in a STEM discipline. They coded photos on the extent to which students exhibited physical features considered stereotypical for their races (white, black, or Asian).

Using logistic regression analysis, the researchers found that Asian students who looked more (versus less) racially stereotypical were significantly more likely to complete a bachelor’s degree in STEM. However, black students who looked more racially stereotypical were less likely to complete a STEM degree. The findings reported no meaningful differences for white students.

Image: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

“I think we live in a presumed meritocracy where people believe what you get on tests and how you do in the classroom is what matters,” says Mikki Hebl, chair of psychological sciences and professor of management at Rice University. “Our research says that your looks do matter and can impact your likelihood to depart or remain in a STEM field. And that is pretty shocking.”

Tracing the bias

In an attempt to identify the source of such biases, the researchers invited academic advisers from 50 top US universities to participate in a follow-up study. Advisers looked at two photos (one higher and one lower in stereotypicality but always of the same race and gender) and told to recommend which of the two should take a STEM-related class.

The responses were consistent with the findings in the first part of the study. Advisers were considerably more likely to choose Asian male and female students who looked more stereotypically Asian to take the STEM class. Advisers were less likely to choose those who looked more stereotypically black among women.

The researchers found one inconsistency with the earlier findings: The pattern that academic advisers showed for black women did not extend to black men. That is, the advisers were more likely to say that a black male with a more (versus less) stereotypical appearance would be more successful in STEM. Hebl points to heightened sensitivities to black men in the current political culture and movements such as #BlackLivesMatter as a possible cause for this finding. She suggested very stereotypical black men may have led participants to double check their behavior.

“We have some evidence that the extent to which people tried to suppress their motivation to be prejudiced predicted their responses to black men, but only to them,” Hebl says.

When the researchers controlled for the motivation to control prejudice, they found that the pattern for black men resembled the pattern for black women—that is, the less-stereotypical face led to perceptions of greater STEM ability.

Reason for hope

Nevertheless, this finding encourages the researchers.

“We think it may demonstrate that some level of awareness about biases exist, and individuals are capable of altering their actions. Whether it is political correctness, a true change of heart, or something in between, a change of behavior is possible,” Hebl says.

Hebl has been examining the construct of stereotypicality for nearly 10 years. A 2006 Stanford study that showed black men were more likely to get the death penalty if they look more (versus less) racially stereotypical motivated Hebl to begin research to identify other outcomes potentially linked to stereotypicality.

Have you read?

In 2010, she published research that showed one’s level of stereotypicality negatively correlates with the size of that person’s social network. That is, she found that those who look more stereotypical of their race (unless they are white) are less likely to have as large a social network and be as central within it.

The authors of the STEM study say they don’t think society and researchers have done enough to understand how differences within races, versus between races, lead to different outcomes.

“This study is important because it raises awareness about how decisions based on stereotypes can amount to very real negative outcomes for STEM students,” the authors write. “We hope that by bringing attention to this topic, it will create ripples of awareness and behaviors that lead to more authentic, meritocratically based outcomes.”

Don't miss any update on this topic

Create a free account and access your personalized content collection with our latest publications and analyses.

Sign up for free

License and Republishing

World Economic Forum articles may be republished in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International Public License, and in accordance with our Terms of Use.

The views expressed in this article are those of the author alone and not the World Economic Forum.

Stay up to date:

Education

Related topics:
Education and SkillsJobs and the Future of Work
Share:
The Big Picture
Explore and monitor how Education is affecting economies, industries and global issues
A hand holding a looking glass by a lake
Crowdsource Innovation
Get involved with our crowdsourced digital platform to deliver impact at scale
World Economic Forum logo
Global Agenda

The Agenda Weekly

A weekly update of the most important issues driving the global agenda

Subscribe today

You can unsubscribe at any time using the link in our emails. For more details, review our privacy policy.

How 'green education' could speed up the net-zero transition

Sonia Ben Jaafar

November 22, 2024

Systems thinking has great potential in education. Here are 5 ways to deliver it

About us

Engage with us

  • Sign in
  • Partner with us
  • Become a member
  • Sign up for our press releases
  • Subscribe to our newsletters
  • Contact us

Quick links

Language editions

Privacy Policy & Terms of Service

Sitemap

© 2024 World Economic Forum