Sustainable Development

This Lego experiment shows our brains prefer adding. Here's why it matters

image of some Lego bricks

Understanding the human preference for addition (as opposed to subtraction) could be beneficial for sustainability. Image: Unsplash/Fran Jacquier

Harry Kretchmer
Senior Writer, Forum Agenda
  • New research shows that people prefer addition over subtraction.
  • Academics used Lego to show that people tend to add things even when it’s against their interests.
  • This may be because taking things away requires more cognitive effort.
  • The findings could help us understand our impact on the planet and design better technology.

Many of us have drawn a picture and added just one more line or detail, only to realize - too late - we’ve ruined it.

This tendency to add things (and overlook the benefits of removing other things) may be our default state - even when it costs us money, according to University of Virginia researchers, who have demonstrated it using Lego.

The academics say their findings, published in Nature, could help explain why many people find it hard to manage hectic workloads and may be why humanity is damaging the planet.

Have you read?

As projects like UpLink search for ideas to boost sustainability, figuring out why we’re biased towards accumulation could lead to technology that understands us better.

image of a roof being added to a lego-based structure
To stabilize this roof, would you remove one block or add several blocks? Image: University of Virginia

What has the research found?

Participants were given a variety of tasks, including working out how best to stabilize the top of a Lego structure. They could do this either by adding new bricks - at a cost - or simply by removing a single brick.

Despite subtracting a brick being the simplest - and cheapest - strategy, the researchers discovered that under control conditions, around 60% of people would add more bricks anyway.

Unless participants were prompted, they were less likely to spot when taking things away might be to their advantage. And they were more likely to add things when time was limited, or when they were given several things to think about.

Across eight experiments, the researchers found that people “systematically default to searching for additive transformations, and consequently overlook subtractive transformations”. In other words, we tend to add things, regardless of whether it’s the best strategy.

a graph depicting people's tendency to choose 'subjective ideas'
Some experiments showed people had a greater tendency to choose ‘subtractive ideas’ (shown in blue) if prompted. Image: Nature

So what’s going on?

We’re often told that less is more. Marie Kondo’s decluttering technique has inspired people around the world to live simpler lives. And increasing numbers of businesses recognize the benefits of a less resource-intensive circular economy.

Yet from our workplaces, to the kitchen, to the supermarket shop, our default solution is often to propose another meeting, add another pinch of salt, or buy an extra item. The explanation lies in how our brains are wired.

In the 1950s, the economist Herbert A. Simon proposed that humans have ‘bounded rationality’: we often deal with situations by reaching for an accessible solution, rather than the best one. Behavioural scientists including Nobel Prize-winner, Richard Thaler, have developed the idea that intuition drives much of our, often flawed, behaviour.

The University of Virginia’s researchers have added to our understanding of what this flawed behavior makes us do. "Additive ideas come to mind quickly and easily, but subtractive ideas require more cognitive effort," report co-author Benjamin Converse explained to Science Daily. "Because people are often moving fast and working with the first ideas that come to mind, they end up accepting additive solutions without considering subtraction at all."

Discover

Financing Sustainable Development

Can we change?

The good news is that it may be possible for us to gain some control over our addition-driven instincts.

The researchers found that people could be influenced into making more careful choices. When the Lego experiment’s participants were told that “removing pieces is free”, more people took blocks away than added them.

Such ‘nudges’ have already been used positively during COVID-19. For example, some public campaigns have emphasized how handwashing can be good for the whole community.

The researchers behind the University of Virginia also spot potential. The report’s co-author, Leidy Klotz, is optimistic: "I think our research has tremendous implications across contexts,” he says, “especially in engineering to improve how we design technology to benefit humanity."

Loading...
Don't miss any update on this topic

Create a free account and access your personalized content collection with our latest publications and analyses.

Sign up for free

License and Republishing

World Economic Forum articles may be republished in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International Public License, and in accordance with our Terms of Use.

The views expressed in this article are those of the author alone and not the World Economic Forum.

Stay up to date:

Sustainable Development

Related topics:
Sustainable DevelopmentClimate ActionHealth and Healthcare Systems
Share:
The Big Picture
Explore and monitor how Sustainable Development is affecting economies, industries and global issues
A hand holding a looking glass by a lake
Crowdsource Innovation
Get involved with our crowdsourced digital platform to deliver impact at scale
World Economic Forum logo
Global Agenda

The Agenda Weekly

A weekly update of the most important issues driving the global agenda

Subscribe today

You can unsubscribe at any time using the link in our emails. For more details, review our privacy policy.

Extended producer responsibility and a global plastics treaty – what do the experts say?

Jeet Kar, Madeleine Sophia Brandes and Audrey Helstroffer

November 18, 2024

How regenerative agriculture can make climate solutions more resilient

About us

Engage with us

  • Sign in
  • Partner with us
  • Become a member
  • Sign up for our press releases
  • Subscribe to our newsletters
  • Contact us

Quick links

Language editions

Privacy Policy & Terms of Service

Sitemap

© 2024 World Economic Forum